Foveal vs. Parafoveal Processing in AnxietyBroadened Spatial Attention for Threat Words

  1. Gutiérrez García, Aida
  2. Gutiérrez Calvo, Manuel
Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Año de publicación: 2011

Volumen: 32

Número: 2

Páginas: 301-321

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumen

We investigated the processing of threat-related, positive, and neutral words in parafoveal and in foveal vision as a function of individual differences in trait anxiety. In a lexical-decision task, word primes were presented for 150 ms either parafoveally (2.2° away from fixation; Experiment 1) or foveally (at fixation; Experiment 3) followed by a probe word; or a foveal probe word was presented alone (with no prime; Experiment 2). Results showed that parafoveal prime threat words facilitated responses to probe threat words for high-anxiety individuals, in comparison with neutral and positive words, and relative to low-anxiety individuals. In contrast, when the words were presented foveally, there were no differences in resting activation level (i.e., accessibility to single word meanings) or firing thresholds (i.e., foveal priming) as a function of emotional content and anxiety. This reveals a covert attention bias towards threat stimuli in anxiety.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Allen, P.A., Smith, A.F., Lien, M.C., Grabbe, J., & Murphy, M.D. (2005). Evidence for an activation locus of the word-frequency effect in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 713-721.
  • Balota, D.A., Cortese, M.J., Sergent-Marshall, S.D., Spieler, D.H., Yap, M.J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283-316.
  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24.
  • Battista, J., & Kalloniatis, M. (2002). Left-right word recognition asymmetries in central and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 42, 1583-1592.
  • Bradley, B.P., Mogg, K., & Williams, R. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory for emotional information in non-clinical subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 65-78.
  • Calvo, M.G., & Castillo, M.D. (2009). Semantic word priming in the absence of eye fixations: Relative contribution of overt and covert attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 51-56.
  • Calvo, M.G., Castillo, M.D., & Fuentes, L.J. (2006). Processing of "unattended" threatrelated information: Role of emotional content and context. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 785-811.
  • Calvo, M.G., & Eysenck, M.W. (2008). Affective significance enhances covert attention: Roles of anxiety and word familiarity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1669-1686.
  • Calvo, M.G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2009). Word identification by covert attention in the right visual field: Gaze-contingent foveal masking. Laterality, 14, 178-195.
  • Clore, G.L., & Huntsinger, J.R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 393-399.
  • Davis, C. J., & Perea, M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 665-671.
  • Easterbrook, J.A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilisation and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183-201.
  • Eysenck, M.W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336-353.
  • Findlay, J.M., & Gilchrist, I. (2003). Active vision: The psychology of looking and seeing. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Fox, E. (1993). Attentional bias in anxiety: Selective or not? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 487-493.
  • Fox, E. (1994). Attentional bias in anxiety: A defective inhibition hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 165-195.
  • Fox, E., & Georgiou, G. A. (2005). The nature of attentional bias in human anxiety. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology (pp. 249-274). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700.
  • Fredrickson, B., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertories. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.
  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G.L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global vs. local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34-40.
  • Hyona, J., & Koivisto, M. (2006). The role of eye movements in lateralised word recognition. Laterality, 11, 155-169.
  • Kanne, S. M. (2002). The role of semantic, orthographic, and phonological prime information in unilateral visual neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 245-261.
  • Keogh, E., & French, C.C. (1999). The effect of trait anxiety and mood manipulation on the breadth of attention. European Journal of Personality, 13, 209-223.
  • Lachter, J., Forster, K.I., & Ruthruff, E. (2004). Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): Still no identification without attention. Psychological Review, 111, 880-913.
  • Lindell, A. K. (2006). In your right mind: Right hemisphere contributions to human language processing and production. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 131-148.
  • Lindell, A. K., Arend, I., Ward, R., Norton, J., & Wathan, J. (2007). Hemispheric asymmetries in feature integration during visual word recognition. Laterality, 12, 543-558.
  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1991). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: Accessibility of information or assignment of processing priorities? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 599-610.
  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 539-560.
  • Mayer, B., & Merkelbach, H. (1999). Unconscious processes, subliminal stimulation, and anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 571-590.
  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809-848.
  • Pacheco-Unguetti, A.P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., & Lupianez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety: Different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298-304.
  • Pacheco-Unguetti. A.P, Lupianez, J., & Acosta, A. (2009). Atencion y ansiedad: Relaciones de la alerta y el control cognitivo de la ansiedad rasgo. Psicológica, 30, 1-25.
  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372-422.
  • Robinson, M. D. (1998). Running from William James' bear: A review of preattentive mechanisms and their contribution to emotional experience. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 667-696.
  • Rusting, C.L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 165-196.
  • Shapiro, K.L., & Lim, A. (1989). The impact of anxiety on visual attention to central and peripheral events. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 345-351.
  • Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Lushene, R.E. (1982). Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado-Rasgo [State Trait Anxiety Inventory]. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Staal, M.A. (2004). Stress, cognition, and human performance: A literature review and conceptual framework. Hanover, MD: NASA.
  • Wandell, B.A. (1995). Foundations of vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
  • Wu, S.-C., & Remington, R. (2003). Characteristics of covert and overt visual orienting: Evidence from attentional and oculomotor capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1050-1067.